SALMON: Study And Learning Materials ONline
The role of nature and nurture in the development of behaviour

Welcome to SALMON the 1998 UCISA award winning website developed by Dr Paul Kenyon (retired) to support students studying evolutionary psychology and behavioral neuroscience in the Department of Psychology, University of Plymouth, Devon, UK

I am now retired and run Fly Fishing Devon: Instruction & Guiding on Dartmoor & South Devon Rivers

Overview

This lecture focuses on the work of ethologists who were primarily interested in the evolution of animal behaviour.

bard.gif (15610 bytes)An important aspect of this topic is to alert you to the nature - nurture debate which has political as well as scientific ramifications. It emphasizes once again that science is not pursued in a vacuum; it occurs within a social, political and cultural context.

In The Tempest by William Shakespeare, Pospero refers to Caliban as :

"A devil, a born devil, on whose nature Nurture can never stick;"

By the end of the topic you should appreciate that behaviour is not simply the result of the unfolding of a genetic blueprint, nor the expression of environmental influences, instead it is a product of the interaction between these two powerful forces.

Learning objectives

After studying the material on this page you should be able to:
  • recognize the names & faces of the founders of ethology and behaviourism
  • list and compare the characteristics of ethology and experimental animal psychology
  • list and explain the four questions asked by ethologists
  • describe the courtship behaviour of male sticklebacks
  • describe the 'nature/nurture' problem
  • explain the significance of Hailman's experiment on development of pecking preference in young herring gulls
  • describe the interaction between innate and environmental factors in the development of bird song
  • understand the role of practice and maturation in the development of behaviour
  • describe preparedness and taste aversion learning
  • Criticize the view that behaviour is either nature or nurture
  • Looking for something else? Insert terms below to carry out a Google search of SALMON

    Ethologists

    Konrad Lorenz

    Lorenz
    Niko Tinbergen
    Tinbergen

    Founding fathers of ethology and behaviourism

    After the second world war there were two broad approaches to the sLtudy of animal behaviour in Europe and America.

    The European school was founded in the 1930's by the Austrian Konrad Lorenz . He collaborated with the Dutch zoologist Niko Tinbergen to establish 'ethology' which he defined as the 'biological study of behaviour'. Tinbergen's book 'The Study of Instinct' remains the best introduction to the ethological approach to the study of animal behaviour.

    The American approach to animal behaviour has its roots in the work of J.B. Watson who in 1924 laid the foundation for an experimental approach to the study of behaviour in his book 'Behaviourism'. Watson was influenced by Pavlov's work on classical conditioning, and the English philosopher John Locke who believed that we are born as a blank slate "tabula rasa" on to which we write the associations we perceive in our environment.

    Watson's ideas were adopted by experimental psychologists who were particularly interested in studying learning under laboratory conditions. Perhaps the best known exponent of this approach in its purest form was Fred Skinner who believed that behaviour was shaped by reward. Essentially reward leads to the repetition of a behaviour.

    Behaviourist

    Fred Skinner

    The rat's behaviour is 'shaped' by giving a pellet of food delivered via a button in Skinner's hand.

    Characteristics of ethology and comparative psychology

    Ethologists are concerned with:
  • identifying and describing species-specific behaviours. These are behaviours that show little variability between members of the same species e.g. courtship displays in birds
  • understanding the evolutionary pathway through which the genetic basis for the behaviour came about.
  • Many ethologists capture their observations on videotape or audio tape. From their observations they make an ethogram: a description and documentation of the behavioural patterns under study, a behavioural inventory.
  • The first ethologists were European scientists (e.g. Lorenz, Tinbergen)
  • trained in zoology;
  • studied the evolution of behaviour;
  • in birds, fish and insects;
  • used field experiments and made observations of animal behaviour under natural conditions;
  • discovered species specific fixed action patterns elicited by sign stimuli acting through innate releasing mechanisms;
  • used the term instinct to explain motivation
  • Behaviourists and comparative psychologists were:

  • initially North American scientists,
  • trained in psychology,
  • rejected the notion of instinct
  • interested in the flexibility of behaviour shown by individuals rather than the evolution of behaviour in species, and
  • understanding the environmental requirements for the development of behaviour in the young;
  • Studied how we learn new behaviours,
  • using a restricted number of species, principally rats and pigeons,
  • under laboratory conditions, using statistical methods and carefully controlled experimental variables,
  • with the intention of discovering general laws of behaviour that could be applied to all species including humans.
  • Queen Victoria and family

    Point to ponder:

    What relationship (if any) is there between the scientific approaches adopted by these European and American researchers and the political and cultural systems on the two continent

    President Abraham Lincoln

    The four questions ethologists ask about behaviour

    Consider the courtship behaviour of the male three-spined stickleback described by Tinbergen (1966). This appears to be innate behaviour: a sequence of fixed action patterns shown by all males in breeding condition, each behaviour triggered by a specific external stimulus . When a ripe female swollen with eggs enters his territory, the male darts towards - and away from - the female in a so-called zigzag dance. The female is led by the male to a nest he has constructed on the floor of the pond or stream. She may creep through the nest and spawn. The male then follows and fertilizes the eggs. He may chase the female away. The male stays by the nest periodically fanning the nest to drive water over it and oxygenate the eggs. Here are several pictures showing this sequence of behaviours.

    Courtship behaviour of the male three-spined stickleback

    1

    In spring, male sticklebacks come into breeding condition indicated by their bright red bellies and iridescent blue on the back, and build a nest of weed stuck together with sticky kidney fluid.

    2

    A female- belly swollen with eggs - elicits the male's 'zigzag' dance which terminates in the male sticking his head into the nest entrance.

    3

    The female enters the nest.

    4

    . The male hovers over the female, prodding the base of her tail which ...

    5

    . .. induces the female to lay her eggs.

    6

    The male enters the nest and ...

    7

    . .. may drive the female out. She plays no further part in looking after nest, eggs or young. These duties are performed by the male.

    8

    The male enters the nest, fertilizes the eggs and leaves the nest.

    9

    The male repairs the nest, mates with two or three other females, and oxygenates the eggs by 'fanning'.

    10

    The male remains with the nest for about 7 days until the eggs hatch. After hatching, he protects the offspring by picking up strays and spitting them back into the nest.

    Ethologists asked four questions about this type of behaviour.
    How has it evolved and developed?
    What causes it, and what is its function?
    One way of thinking about these questions is to consider them as points on a species' journey through time, from the distant past into the future.

  • The evolution question: How has courtship behaviour evolved in the species? Is it shown by other species of sticklebacks?
  • The development question: How does courtship behaviour develop during the individual's lifetime? Does the male learn the zigzag dance? Does he practice dancing? Is the male successful in directing the female to the nest the first time he dances?
  • The cause question: What causes the male to dance? Are there internal factors that increase the male's tendency to dance? Does increasing daylength in Spring increase the males tendency to dance? Does the swollen belly of a female elicit dancing in males?
  • The function question: Why does the male dance in a particular way? Why does the male prod the female once she has entered the nest? Why does the male fan the eggs? How do these behaviours contribute to the reproductive success of the individual, and ultimately the survival of the species?
  • Tension between ethology and psychology

    It is perhaps not surprising that ethologists and behaviourists would eventually clash over their very different approaches to recording, analysing and interpreting behaviour (e.g. Lehrman. (1953)..

    After all they were studying very different types of behaviour.

    For example, whilst ethologists were observing courtship displays in the field, psychologists were poring over cumulative records showing the impact of schedules of reinforcement on rates of bar-pressing in rats trained in Skinner boxes under carefully controlled laboratory conditions.

    One flash point was interpretation of how behaviour develops.

    According to Lorenz, species-specific behaviour develops without the animal experiencing the stimuli to which it responds, or without practice of the motor patterns that it performs. Terms associated with this view that behaviour is the result of 'nature' include:

  • instinctive behaviour
  • innate behaviour
  • inherited behaviour
  • genetic bases of behaviour
  • The American John Watson is credited with emphasizing the role of nurture in development. He wrote

    "give me a dozen healthy infants, well -formed, and my own specified world to bring them up in and I'll guarantee to take anyone at random and train them to become any type of specialist I might select - doctor, lawyer, artist, merchant-chief, and, yes, even beggar man and thief , regardless of his talents, penchants, tendencies, abilities, vocations, and race of his ancestors" (see Boakes, 1984, pp226).

    Watson was trying to develop a psychology that could be utilized by "the educator, the physician, the jurist and the business man ... in a practical way"

    Terms associated with the view that behaviour is the result of 'nurture' include:
  • acquired behaviour
  • learned behaviour
  • environment
  • We are now going to examine several representative studies that served to break down the notion that development was an either or process; experiments that debunked the notion that behaviour was either the result of nature or purely the result of nurture; experiments that blurred the lines between nature and nurture, and that laid the foundation for the modern view that behaviour develops as an interaction between factors in an animal's environment, as well as biological predispositions.

    Nature and nurture in the development of behaviour

    Hailman's experiment on development of pecking preference in young herring gulls

    Herring gull chicks peck at a red spot on their parents' bill to induce them to regurgitate food. Hailman tested Lorenz's claim that this behaviour is innate.

    This diagram indicates that at birth herring gull chicks peck equally often at a model of their own species, and at a model of a laughing gull, but after 6 days of experience receiving food from their parents they show a preference for the model of their own species.

    This study shows that the tendency to peck is probably innate, but the object that is pecked is modified as a result of experience

    Marler's work on the development of bird song

    A sonogram is a visual representation of bird song. If your computer has a sound card you can click on this picture of a sonogram to hear the bird song

    The development of bird song illustrates how genetic and environmental factors interact during the development of a behaviour. Because bird song in passerines is learnt from others, then song development is an example of culture.

    White crowned sparrows have geographically stable dialects.

    This diagram shows the similarities in the sonograms of two geographically dispersed subspecies of white crowned sparrow :

  • Nuttal's white crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophyrs nuttalli) which breeds along the central Californian coast
  • Mountain white crown sparrows Zonotrichia leucophyrs oriantha which breed high in the Californian Sierra Nevada mountains
  • For comparison, a sonogram from a swamp sparrow (Melospiza georgiana) a different species of sparrow which lives in Pennsylvania is shown in the diagram.
  • Within the same species, there are regional variations in bird song. Although these differences could be interpreted as evidence for a genetic basis for bird song, research has shown that young birds learn the dialect from adults in their area.

  • If infant birds are raised in sound proofed chambers in a laboratory, they emit a crude but recognizable song that contains elements from the normal song. Marler explained this by postulating that young birds are born with a crude template of what their species song should sound like. They match this template to the song they hear around them during development, so that the template is sharpened.
  • When the bird is isolated during this memorization phase, all it can produce is the crude template.
  • The song of a deafened bird is even cruder than that produced by the isolated bird because although the deafened bird may have an exact template, it cannot hear its own output, so it cannot compare the song that it sings with the internal template.
    The white crowned sparrow may be unusual in having a sensitive period for memorization that is over before the bird itself begins to sing. Other birds may show a longer period during which they learn a song. See Chapter 3 in Halliday & Slater (1983)
  • From 10 to 50 days of age, the young male's template accepts this adult male white crowned sparrow song as a model:


    male white crowned sparrow song
    and rejects this swamp swallow song as a model:
    swamp swallow song

    The improved template now specifies the dialect he has to learn. The young bird does not sing, but the model is remembered for two months or more

    The maturing male begins singing its sub song at about 150 days of age. During this period vocal output is gradually matched to the dialect specified by the improved template
    improved template

    At about 200 days of age full song begins, it is a copy of the model he learned in his youth:
    full song

    From 10 to 50 days of age, no acceptable model is provided to the young male so his template remains crude

    The bird cannot learn a dialect. The young bird does not sing and retains its basic unimproved template for two months or more

    The maturing male begins sub song (about 150 days). Vocal output develops to match specifications of the unimproved template. No dialect, but some species qualities persist
    unimproved template

    Full song begins, based on unimproved template (about 200 days)
    Full song based on unimproved template

    The young male is deafened before he begins sub song His template is inaccessible for vocal development

    The bird cannot learn a dialect. The young bird does not sing and retains its basic unimproved template for two months or more

    The maturing male begins sub song (about 150 days). Vocal output cannot be matched to the unimproved template. No dialect, and species qualities are lost
    Full song begins, based on unimproved template (about 200 days)
    unimproved template

    Full song begins, based on unimproved template (about 200 days)
    unimproved template

    Summary of development of bird song

    Marler's experiments clearly show the interaction between innate and environmental factors.
  • Sparrows have dialects.
  • Isolated birds show deficiencies in their song but it does contain elements of adult song. Marler believes this is because bird is born with a template of what their species song sounds like.
  • Isolated birds can only produce this crude template song.
  • The song of a deafened bird is even cruder.
  • Normal birds adjust their song to what it hears from adults in the vicinity.
  • Point to ponder: What are the implications of these results for human development?

    Wells' experiments on sepia attack behaviour


    Here is an experiment which shows that learning can take place by simply practising a response without any reinforcement.

    The response of young cuttlefish to a tiny shrimp (Mysis) presented in a glass tube can be divided into the four stages shown in this animation:

    1. a latency before there is any observable response
    2. the nearest eye of the cuttlefish fixates the shrimp
    3. the cuttlefish turns towards the shrimp and both eyes fixate
    4. it attacks and would seize the shrimp if it was not behind a glass wall

    Results

    Stages 2, 3 and 4 usually take about 10 seconds and this time varies very little with age and experience.

    But the duration of stage 1 shows a rapid decline with successive tests as shown in this figure. After 5 trials at the rate of one per day, the latency is reduced from about 120 seconds to 10 seconds or less.

    This change is the same whether the attacks are successful or unsuccessful, whether made by a one day old cuttlefish or one starved for 5 days before its first test.

    Wells' experiment shows that practice without reinforcement can lead to a change in behaviour. He found that the latency for the cuttlefish to attack a shrimp declined with practice even though the cuttlefish was not reinforced. Note that hunger (increased motivation) did not improve performance of this behaviour.

    The only common element appears to be practice in attacking shrimps. Reinforcement does not appear to play a role in the acquisition of this response. 

    Maturation and practice of pecking by chicks


    The development of pecking in newly hatched chicks is an example of the interaction between maturation and practice in the development of a behaviour. Newly hatched chicks have an inherited tendency to peck at objects which contrast with their background, at first their aim is poor but it does improve. Cruze studied how this improvement occurs. He measured pecking accuracy by testing chicks individually in a small arena with a black floor onto which he scattered several grains of millet. Each chick was allowed 25 pecks; each peck was scored as a hit or miss.

    Experimental design: Cruze used the following experimental design:

    The experiment involved nine independent groups of chicks:
  • Tested after being kept in dark for 24 hours with no opportunity to practice
  • Tested after being kept in dark for 48 hours with no opportunity to practice
  • Kept in dark for 36 hours, given 12 hours in light to practice pecking, then tested when 48 hours old
  • Tested after being kept in dark for 72 hours with no opportunity to practice
  • Kept in dark for 60 hours, given 12 hours in light to practice pecking, then tested when 72 hours old
  • Tested after being kept in dark for 96 hours with no opportunity to practice
  • Kept in dark for 84 hours, given 12 hours in light to practice pecking, then tested when 96 hours old
  • Tested after being kept in dark for 120 hours with no opportunity to practice
  • Kept in dark for 108 hours, given 12 hours in light to practice pecking, then tested when 120 hours old
  • (Chicks were hand fed whilst in the dark)
  • Results:

  • As the chicks mature (age) there is a steady improvement in accuracy even though they have not had any opportunity to practice pecking.
  • At any age, 12 hours of practice greatly improves accuracy.
  • Conclusion:

    pecking improves as a consequence of both maturation and practice.

    Limits to behaviourism:

    Preparedness and taste aversion learning

    Behaviourists such as Skinner gave the impression that it was possible to condition any response that an animal could perform. However, the idea that through the process of operant conditioning, any reinforcer should be equally effective in increasing the frequency of any response was soon in serious doubt.

    For example, although it is fairly easy to train a rat to run in a wheel to avoid shock, it proved impossible to condition a rat to rear (stand upright) to avoid the aversive stimulus. Seligman argued that evolution had prepared animals to make certain associations more easily than others. Thus rats are 'prepared' to run, but not to stand on their back legs, to avoid or escape from an unpleasant stimulus (data redrawn from Bolles, 1973)

    Garcia and Koelling (1966) carried out a famous experiment on taste aversion learning involving 'bright noisy water' and illness induced by exposure to X-radiation. The training and testing conditions in their experiment are described in this table.

    Results:

    The results show that rats did form an association between

  • external cues ('bright noisy water') and shock
  • a distinctive taste (saccharin) and illness
  • But, rats did not form an association between

  • external cues ('bright noisy water') and illness
  • a distinctive taste (saccharin) and shock
  • According to traditional behaviourists, all the groups of rats should have learned an association between drinking from the spout and the aversive consequences, and should not have drunk under the test conditions. Therefore, Garcia's results challenge the idea that any reinforcer is equally effective in increasing the frequency of any response

    Problems with viewing behaviour as either nature or nurture

    According to the protagonists, behaviour can be divided into two types

    Conclusions:

  • Simple "nature/nurture" or "instinctive/learned" dichotomies have now been abandoned
  • All behaviour depends on both genes and environment
  • Attention is now focussed on experimental investigation of what does, or does not, influence behavioural development
  • Nowadays ethologists and psychologists are less divided by the nature - nurture debate. Both groups are interested in how genetic and environmental factors interact to control the development and expression of behaviour. In turn an individual's behaviour and environment feedback into the organism to determine its future behaviour.
  • Point to ponder
    Can you think of any recent debates about the causes of human behaviour which have been influenced by the nature - nurture debate? Can you classify the participants as taking a 'nature' or 'nurture' or 'interactions' view of human behaviour?

    References and online resources

    Here is a list of books that extend the topics discussed on this page. You do not need to read all of them. They are included here to give you a choice of further readings.

  • Ariew, Andre (1999) Innateness Is Canalization: In Defense of a Developmental Account of Innateness,in Hardcastle, Valerie, Eds. Biology Meets Psychology: Conjectures, Connections, Constraints. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
    • for a reaction to Ariew, see  Griffiths P.E. What is Innateness?To appear in The Monist, Special Issue, edited by Kim Sterelny
  • Boakes (1984). From Darwinism to behaviourism. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
  • Bolles (1973). The comparative psychology of learning: The selection association principle and some problems with "general" laws of learning. In Perspectives in Animal Behavior. Bermant (Ed). Scott, Foresman & Company, Glenville, IL.
  • Dewsbury (1978) Comparative animal behavior. McGraw Hill, New York.
  • Dickinson (1980). Contemporary animal learning theory, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
  • Garcia, Kimmeldorf & Koelling, (1955). Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science, 122, 157-158.
  • Garcia and Koelling (1966).Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science, 4, 123-124.
  • Halliday & Slater (Eds.) (1983). An Introduction to Animal Behaviour: Vol3 Genes, Development & Learning, Blackwell Scientific Publications.
  • Hamer, Hubbard and Wright (1998) debate the nature-nurture question in a Talk of the Nation radio programmebroadcast on June 25, 1998
  • Hinde ((1982). Ethology, Oxford University Press, Oxford.
  • Hinde (1970). Animal behaviour: A synthesis of ethology and comparative psychology, McGraw Hill, New York.
  • Klein (2000). The ethological approach to the study of human behavior. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 21, 477-481.
  • Lehrman, D. S. (1953). A critique of Konrad Lorenz's theory of instinctive behavior. Quarterly Review of Biology 28:337-363.
  • Manning and Dawkins (1998). An Introduction to Animal Behaviour, 5th Edition, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
  • Ridley (1986). Animal Behaviour: a concise introduction, Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford.
  • Shakespeare (written1610-11 ) The Tempest. Act IV Scene 1 line 188 available online
  • Slater (1999). Essentials of animal behaviour, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
  • Tinbergen (1966). Animal Behaviour. Time-Life International, Nederland, N.V.


  • Here is a link to an article by Charles T. Snowdon, past President of the Animal Behavior Society on the Significance of Animal Behavior Research
    Abstract from introduction:

    "Animal behaviour is the bridge between the molecular and physiological aspects of biology and the ecological. Behavior is the link between organisms and environment and between the nervous system, and the ecosystem. Behavior is one of the most important properties of animal life. Behavior plays a critical role in biological adaptations. Behavior is how we humans define our own lives. Behavior is that part of an organism by which it interacts with its environment. Behavior is as much a part of an organisms as its coat, wings etc. The beauty of an animal includes its behavioural attributes. 

    For the same reasons that we study the universe and subatomic particles there is intrinsic interest in the study of animals. In view of the amount of time that television devotes to animal films and the amount of money that people spend on nature books there is much more public interest in animal behaviour than in neutrons and neurons. If human curiosity drives research, then animal behaviour should be near the top of our priorities. 

    While the study of animal behaviour is important as a scientific field on its own, our science has made important contributions to other disciplines with applications to the study of human behaviour, to the neurosciences, to the environment and resource management, to the study of animal welfare and to the education of future generations of scientists. "


  • An extensive list of sites covering Animal behaviour: Internet Resource Guide for Zoology 
  • the Nebraska Behavioral Biology Groupmaintains an extensive list of Internet sites that pertain to animal behaviour 
  • the International Society for Human Ethologyhas links to web pages covering the ethology and evolution 
  • a Web-site for the academic study of bird song
  • Film Archive of Human Ethologyfounded by Prof. Dr. Eibl-Eibesfeldt and Prof. Dr. Hass