What do we mean by the terms sex and gender?
Sex
is a biological term which refers to the functional
differences between males and females and their reproductive potential
Gender is a psychological term which refers to our awareness and reaction to biological sex
|
Biological
and psychological variables that affect gender
|
![]() |
Gender consists of
several elements:
What do you think is the gender role, gender identity and sexual orientation of these two characters played by Jack Lemmon and Tony Curtis in the 1959 film Some Like it Hot? |
The development of gender identity: Nature or Nurture?
Each of us has a gender
identity - a
private feeling that we are male or female. There are two fundamentally
different
explanations for how this develops.
The most
influential theory is that our gender is the
result of environmental influences, particularly the way we are treated
by our parents,
guardians, friends and relatives. According to Dr John Money we are psychosexually
neutral at birth, and our gender is a consequence of the nurture
we receive as
children.
The less popular view is that gender may is the result of nature, particularly the effects of hormones on the developing brain.
This is not a dry academic argument. The lives of a significant number of people have been changed as a result of the application of these theories.
Point to ponder:
|
This web page describes some of the evidence that suggests that gender may the result of exposure to hormones in the first few weeks of life.
Development of male and female external genitalia
Perhaps the first question asked by parents and relatives after a child is born is "What sex is the child?". According to the nurture view of psychosexual differentiation this is the point at which shaping the child's gender identity begins. But the process may have started many weeks before when the baby was developing in the mother's womb. This series of diagrams shows how the development of a child's external genitalia is affected by exposure to the androgen dihydrotestosterone.
An important message from this diagram is that - regardless of genetic sex -
![]() |
In early intrauterine development male and female external genitalia (visible sex organs) are identical. |
![]() |
Under the influence of the androgen dihydrotestosterone, the external genitalia develop in the male direction. In the absence of androgens female external genitalia develop. |
![]() |
The crucial point about
this diagram is that tissues that are equivalent for the two sexes in
the undifferentiated state will become different organs by the time of
birth. You can follow this change by tracing the fates of the different
coloured tissue. The top diagram shows the undifferentiated state. The
middle diagrams shows differentiation beginning during the third and
fourth months of pregnancy. The bottom diagrams show complete
differentiation of the external genitalia at birth.
These diagrams are based on the drawings found in most textbooks that cover the the biological bases of sexual behaviour. |
Here is an animated diagram of the effects of androgen exposure on external genital development that will open in a new browser window.
![]() |
The 'Guevedoces' of the Dominican Republic. "During the early 1970s, Dr. Julianne Imperato, a Cornell endocrinologist, conducted an expedition to the Dominican Republic to investigate reports of an isolated village where children appearing to be girls turned into men at puberty. In the village, these children were known as 'guevedoces' (literally, penis at 12 years)." Their underlying pathology was found to be a deficiency of the enzyme 5-alpha Reductase which converts testosterone into 5 dihydrotestosterone |
Activational
and organizational effects of hormones
The nature and nurture views of psychosexual development differ in the significance they attach to the importance of hormones in the development of behavioural differences between males and females.
Hormones have two fundamentally different effects on sexual behaviour: | |
Organizational effects refer to the effects of hormones during the early development of an animal | ![]() ![]() |
Activational effects refer to the effects of hormones in the adult organism | ![]() |
There is evidence that exposure to hormones during a critical period of development changes the way in which the organism reacts to hormones in adulthood. Notice that this does not mean that early exposure to hormones has a permanent effect on behaviour. Instead it suggests that exposure to hormones in infancy affects how the adult reacts to hormones.
In other words, early exposure to hormones organizes the way behaviour is activated by hormones in adulthood. We need to spend a few moments reviewing how the activational effects of hormones on behaviour are measured.
Hormones such as testosterone, estrogen and progesterone, activate sexual behaviour of adult male and female rats
Many
studies measure female sexual
behaviour in terms of 'lordosis'. Lordosis refers to
a characteristic posture in
which the female rodent arches her back and moves her tail to permit
penetration by the
male.
Sexual behaviour in male rats consists of three behaviours:
The activational effects of hormones are discussed in greater detail on a separate page of this website.
This page focusses on the organizational effects of hormones.
According to the nature theory of psychosexual differentiation, hormones organize the brain during development. In a nutshell:
Castration
of male rats in infancy causes them to become:
Treatment of female rats with testosterone in infancy causes them to become:
|
The organizational effects of the presence or absence of testosterone are only apparent in adulthood when the organism is under the activational effects of testosterone or estrogen and progesterone.
Feminization of
male rat by castration in infancy
Exposure
to testosterone between about day 17 of gestation to day 8-10 of
postnatal life organizes
the brain of a male rat to determine how it will react to hormones in
adulthood.
If a male rat is castrated at birth, and then given an injection of estrogen in adulthood, it exhibits lordosis, - a female sexual response - when tested with a sexually vigorous male rat.
If a male rat is castrated at birth, and then exposed to a female rat which is in estrus (heat) he will not mate with her.
In contrast, if a normal male is injected with estrogen in adulthood it does not display the lordosis response in the presence of a sexually vigorous male rat. A normal male rat will mate with a female rat in estrus.
Testosterone
is thought to establish male
circuits (masculinization), and inhibit the
development of female brain circuits (defeminization).
Masculinization of female pups by injection of testosterone in infancy
The absence of
testosterone
organizes the brain of a female rat to determine how it will react to
hormones in
adulthood.
If a female rat is injected with testosterone during infancy, and then given an injection of testosterone in adulthood, she will show male sexual responses - mount, intromission and ejaculation behaviours - when tested with a female rat in estrus.
If a female rat is injected with testosterone during infancy, and then injected with estrogen in adulthood, she will not exhibit lordosis behaviour when she is tested with a sexually vigorous male rat.
In contrast, if a normal female rat is injected with testosterone in adulthood, she will not exhibit male sexual behaviours.
A normal female rat in estrus will exhibit lordosis when paired with a sexually vigorous male rat.
Testosterone is thought to establish male circuits (masculinization), and inhibit the development of female brain circuits (defeminization).
In the absence of testosterone brain circuits are feminized and demasculinized.
Summary
of the effects of male castration and female testosterone treatment
Adult sexual
behavior in the rat depends on whether the brain was organized by
gonadal hormones during
the first few days after birth.
Normal adult males display mounting behavior because their brains were subjected to a dose of testosterone from the gonads just after birth. The same effect can be produced in females by injecting the hormone testosterone.
Depriving
male pups of testosterone by
castrating them at birth results in a female brain organization. As
adults these feminized
males, like normal females, display very few attempts to mount, but a
high frequency of
lordosis when mounted.
Behaviours
influenced by hormone exposure in infancy
Testosterone
exposure in infancy affects a range
of behaviours not just reproductive behaviours.
A number of behaviours in rats are effected by
testosterone exposure around
birth. These include:
Exploratory
behaviour is more
extensive among female rats than among male rats. This behaviour is
modified when female
rats are injected with testosterone shortly after birth. The bar chart
shows the frequency
of defecation, which is inversely proportional to exploration, during a
three minute
open-field test. When the females had not been injected with
testosterone after birth
(left and centre groups) their boli count was significantly less than
males. Females that
had been masculinized defecated at the same rate as males.
Effect of testosterone on the brain
We have seen that early exposure to testosterone affects the external genitalia and adult behaviour, but does it also change the brain? This is an important question. There is evidence that the brains of male and female rodents are structurally different, and that this sexual dimorphism is caused by exposure to androgen during a critical period of development.
An area of the hypothalamus at the base of the brain called the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area (SDN-POA) is much larger in male rats than in females. These diagrams show the location of this sexual dimorphism. You can load an animation that explains the relationship between the saggital and coronal views of the brain in a separate browser window.
Sagittal view of rat brain | Coronal section of rat brain | |
Click your mouse on
parts of the brain to reveal their names.
Abbreviations:
|
![]() |
![]() |
These pictures of sections through the preoptic area of the rat brain show that:
The size
of the sexually dimorphic
nucleus is affected by the presence or absence of testosterone during a
critical period
around birth (neonatal) .
![]() Dr Roger Gorski, UCLA |
Are there sex differences in human behaviour?
Prenatal exposure to
androgen could influence the development of gender role
behaviours - behaviours that are typical of one or the other
sex e.g. boys playing with construction toys; girls playing with dolls Berenbaum (1999) has shown clear differences in activities and job interests between adolescent boys and girls. But gender role behaviours may be influenced by the media.
|
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Here is some unpublished
data collected by Corrine Hutt (University of Reading, UK) who observed
aggressive behaviour in pre-school children in a nursery situation.
She found that overall boys were more aggressive than girls; most aggressive acts tended to involve boys fighting with other boys. The amount of aggression by boys directed towards girls and vice versa tended to be relatively low. Corrine argued that this sex difference in aggression was due to masculinization of the brain by testosterone |
![]() |
Do hormones affect psychosexual differentiation in humans?
We
have already
described how androgen masculinizes the
external genitalia of a developing baby.
Sometimes female embryos are exposed to abnormally high levels of androgen before birth. A small number of genetic females are born with ambiguous external genitalia. The most common cause of female pseudohermaphroditism is congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) which occurs in about 1 in 5,000 to 15,000 live births. Congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) also known as the adrenogenital syndrome (AGS).
CAH is a disease that affects the manufacture of the "stress" hormone, cortisol. Cortisol is released into the blood stream from the adrenal gland, a small organ near the kidney.
The diagram shows how a deficit in cortisol release disrupts the normal negative feedback between cortisol and ACTH secretion from the pituitary gland.
In CAH patients a metabolic error causes overproduction of androgens (e.g. testosterone) in the adrenal gland. This androgen leads to partial masculinization of the external genitalia of female patients which is corrected surgically at birth and with artificial cortisol supplements.
A great deal of attention has been given to the consequences of excess androgens on the psychosexual differentiation of these children.
We will
examine some early psychological
studies of these girls (Ehrhardt, 1975) which suggests that exposure to
androgen during
development causes a partial masculinization of human female behaviour.
Ehrhardt's
study of fetally androgenized genetic female children
This
picture shows a baby girl born with congenital
adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) . Her external genitalia have been partially
masculinized
(virilized) as a result of exposure to high levels of testesoterone
whilst in the womb.
Ehrhardt
(1975) studied 17 female CAH
patients: age 4.3 to 19.9 years, most of the girls were in middle
childhood and early
adolescence. The comparison sample (n=11) consisted of the girls'
sisters who did not have
CAH. All the patients were under long-term corrective treatment with
replacement cortisol
and had undergone surgical correction of the external genitalia,
usually in infancy or
early childhood. Interviews with children and their mothers, fathers
and siblings were
tape-recorded. Interview transcripts were rated according to coded
scales to elicit
information about the child's behaviour.
Activity
and aggression in congenital adrenal
hyperplasia patients
Girls with
CAH were more often described as having
high levels of energy expenditure compared to their unaffected
siblings. They also tended
to prefer to play with boys rather than other girls. Although they
tended to start fights
more frequently than their sisters, this difference was not
statistically significant.
Marriage
and motherhood in congenital adrenal hyperplasia patients
This
diagram shows that girls with CAH were not
very interested in playing with dolls, instead they tended to play with
cars, trucks and
blocks; toys that are generally preferred by boys.
They showed little interest in future roles as brides or mothers, but were much more concerned with their careers.
![]() |
Their
relatives described them as being
indifferent to - or avoiding - contact with babies. For example, they
did not participate
in caring for infants at home or go out babysitting.
Gender role preference behaviours in congenital adrenal hyperplasia
patients
Relatives
and girls with CAH describe themselves as
'tomboys' during all of their childhood.
35% of the sample were unsure or said that they might have chosen to be a boy if they could start their lives over again. However, Ehrhardt points out that none of the girls were unsure about their gender identity. They did not feel that they were boys and being a girl did not make them unhappy. In other words - as a group - they did not exhibit gender dysphoria.
Conclusion: CAH appears to have a significant effect on gender role behaviours. Patients exhibit significantly more male-typical behaviours than unaffected siblings.
Sexual
orientation in CAH/AGS patients
Prenatal
exposure to androgen could influence the
development of:
Money, Schwartz & Lewis (1984) asked 30 women born with CAH about their sexual orientation. Their replies are shown in this diagram together with an estimate of the base rate of female homosexuality according to Kinsey 1953. See Carlson for further details.
Zucker et al (1996) review eight studies that have explored sexual orientation in women with CAH.
Zucker et al (1996) found that most women with CAH have a female gender identity. However, significantly more women with CAH live as men than would be expected by chance.
They
conclude that "excessive exposure
to prenatal androgens in women with CAH shifts psychosexual
differentiation to a point
somewhere in between a female-typical pattern and a male typical
pattern."
Points to ponder
|
"Boy raised as girl discovers happiness as a man"
According
to a nature view of
psychosexual differentiation, prenatal exposure to androgen could
influence the
development of gender identity - the feeling an
individual has of being a man or a
woman. In contrast the nurture position holds that
we are psychosexually neutral
at birth and that socialization is
responsible for the development of gender
identity.
In
1972 Money and Ehrhardt reported the
case of a 7 month old baby boy - one of a pair of twins - born in 1963
whose penis was
removed after an operation for circumcision damaged the child's penis.
At 22 months old
the child was surgically reassigned as a girl and brought up according
to the prevailing
view at the time that we are psychosexually neutral at birth. This case
entered the
textbooks and informed medical opinion for several decades because
Money reported that the
child had adapted well as a girl. But long term follow up of this case
by Milton Diamond
paints a very different picture of the success of this application of
the nature
theory of psychosexual differentiation.
Here is a summary of Diamond and Sigmundson's (1997) paper:
Dr Milton Diamond, Hawaii University"This article is a long-term follow-up to a classic case reported in pediatric, psychiatric, and sexological literature. The penis of an XY individual was accidentally ablated and he was subsequently raised as a female. Initially this individual was described as developing into a normally functioning female. The individual, however, was later found to reject this sex of rearing, switched at puberty to living as a male, and has successfully lived as such from that time to the present. The standard in instances of extensive penile damage to infants is to recommend rearing the male as a female. Subsequent cases should, however, be managed in light of this new evidence."
Here is
a newspaper account of the case:
The story of a boy who
was raised as a girl seems to show that gender really is all in the
genes. Despite the efforts of psychiatrists, surgeons and parents, he
never felt happy as a girl and eventually reverted to being a man, got
married and is now living happily.
The man's life history is told as a cautionary tale by Milton Diamond, a sexologist at Hawaii University in Archives of Paediatric Adolescent Medicine. He says that it is the first long-term follow-up of a male with the normal allotment of XY chromosomes who was raised as a female. Dr Diamond says that the case history has implications for any child born with ambiguous sexuality. "Keep your knife away," he says. "Let the kids make a decision when they get older." Michael Bailey, a psychologist at Northwestern University, Illinois, told Science Now, a daily science news service run by Science magazine, that the case was heralded by many as the pinnacle of proof that psycho-social factors could override biological factors in determining gender. Textbooks continued to claim that Joan made a successful adjustment, in spite of contradictory evidence. Dr Diamond's report, says Dr Bailey, "suggests that, if anything, how you're reared matters little". (Text extract from The Times, March 15 1997, by Nigel Hawkes. Pictures from "The boy who was turned into a girl", Horizon, BBC, December 2000) |
References
Supplementary
Reading HEFCE, the funding body for universities and colleges for the UK, has purchased a 3 year licence to IDEAL, the Academic Press online journal library. If you are a member of a UK academic institution (HEFCE funded) you now have full access rights to this online library which enables you to read the full text of articles in Academic Press journals. The following articles in the journal Hormones and Behavior cover topics raised in the lecture in greater depth:
|